全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量
14517

相关文章

更多...

Applying Evolutionary Thinking to the Study of Emotion

DOI: 10.3390/bs3030388

Keywords: emotion, Cannon-Bard, limbic system, functional adaptation, neuroendocrinology, comparative psychology, elicitor, appraisal, affect, motivated behavior

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

This paper argues for invoking evolutionary, functional thinking in analyzing emotions. It suggests that the fitness needs of normal individuals be kept in mind when trying to understand emotional behavior. This point of view is elaborated in sections addressing these topics: defining emotion; applying comparative analysis to the study of emotions; focusing on the elicitors and resulting motivated behaviors mediated by the various affects; recognizing that not all emotions have prominent, distinct facial expressions; acknowledging all of the basic emotions and not just some exemplars; crediting the more sensible Cannon-Bard theory over James-Lange; recognizing the more ancient, fundamental role of the limbic system in emotion compared with that of the neocortex; and analyzing socio-emotional interactions as they occur naturally, not just individual emotional behavior studied under artificial conditions. Describing the various facets and neuroendocrine mechanisms of each basic emotion can provide a framework for understanding the normal and pathological development of each emotion. Such an inventory, or ethogram, would provide a comprehensive list of all of the observable behavioral tendencies of our species.

References

[1]  Kleinginna, P.R., Jr.; Kleinginna, A.M. A categorized list of emotion definitions, with suggestions for a consensual definition. Motiv. Emotion 1981, 5, 345–379, doi:10.1007/BF00992553.
[2]  Lazarus, R.S. Thoughts on the relations between emotion and cognition. In Approaches to Emotion; Scherer, K.R., Ekman, P., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1984; pp. 247–257.
[3]  Zajonc, R.B. On primacy of affect. In Approaches to Emotion; Scherer, K.R., Ekman, P., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1984; pp. 259–270.
[4]  Krolak-Salmon, P.; Hénaff, M.-A.; Vighetto, A.; Bertrand, O.; Mauguière, F. Early amygdala reaction to fear spreading in occipital, temporal, and frontal cortex: A depth electrode erp study in human. Neuron 2004, 42, 665–676, doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00264-8.
[5]  Maslow, A.H.; Lowry, R. Toward a Psychology of Being, 2nd ed. ed.; Van Nostrand: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1968.
[6]  Panksepp, J. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1998.
[7]  Pugh, G.E. The Biological Origin of Human Values; Basic Books New York: New York, NY, USA, 1977.
[8]  Frijda, N.H. The laws of emotion. Am. Psychol. 1988, 43, 349–358, doi:10.1037/0003-066X.43.5.349.
[9]  Manstead, A.S.R. Social psychology of emotion. In Advanced Social Psychology: The State of the Science; Baumeister, R.F., Finkel, E.J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 101–137.
[10]  Van Hooff, J.A.R.A.M. The comparison of facial expression in man and higher primates. In Methods of Inference from Animal to Human Behaviour; Von Cranach, M., Ed.; Aldine: Chicago, IL, USA, 1976; pp. 165–196.
[11]  Butler, A.B.; Hodos, W. Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy: Evolution and Adaptation; Wiley-Liss: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2005.
[12]  West-Eberhard, M.J. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2003.
[13]  Tracy, J.L.; Shariff, A.F.; Cheng, J.T. A naturalist’s view of pride. Emot. Rev. 2010, 2, 163–177, doi:10.1177/1754073909354627.
[14]  Tracy, J.L.; Robins, R.W. The nonverbal expression of pride: Evidence for cross-cultural recognition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 94, 516–530, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.3.516.
[15]  Weisfeld, G.E.; Dillon, L.M. Applying the dominance hierarchy model to pride and shame, and related behaviors. J. Evolut. Psychol. 2012, 10, 15–41, doi:10.1556/JEP.10.2012.1.2.
[16]  Schjelderup-Ebbe, T. Social behavior of birds. In A Handbook of Social Psychology; Murchison, C., Ed.; Clark University Press: Worcester, MA, USA, 1935; pp. 947–972.
[17]  Alexander, R.D. Aggressiveness, territoriality, and sexual behavior in field crickets (orthoptera: Gryllidae). Behaviour 1961, 17, 130–223, doi:10.1163/156853961X00042.
[18]  Izard, C.E. Human Emotions; Plenum: New York, NY, USA, 1977.
[19]  Tomkins, S.S. Affect theory. In Approaches to Emotions; Scherer, K.R., Ekman, P., Eds.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1984.
[20]  Darwin, C. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1998.
[21]  Tangney, J.P.; Miller, R.S.; Flicker, L.; Barlow, D.H. Are shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 70, 1256, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1256.
[22]  Tracy, J.L.; Robins, R.W. The psychological structure of pride: A tale of two facets. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 92, 506–525, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.506.
[23]  Tangney, J.P.; Stuewig, J.; Mashek, D.J. Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007, 58, 345–372, doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145.
[24]  Wong, Y.; Tsai, J. Cultural models of shame and guilt. In The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research; 2007; pp. 209–223.
[25]  Weisfeld, G.E.; Weisfeld, M.B. Does a humorous element characterize embarrassment? Int. J. Humor Res. 2013. in press.
[26]  Lombardo, M.V.; Baron-Cohen, S.; Belmonte, M.K.; Chakrabarti, B. Neural endophenotypes of social behavior in autism. In The Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience; Decety, J., Cacioppo, J.T., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011.
[27]  Storey, A.E.; Walsh, C.J.; Quinton, R.L.; Wynne-Edwards, K.E. Hormonal correlates of paternal responsiveness in new and expectant fathers. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2000, 21, 79–95, doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00042-2.
[28]  Mak, G.K.; Weiss, S. Paternal recognition of adult offspring mediated by newly generated cns neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2010, 13, 753–758, doi:10.1038/nn.2550.
[29]  Taylor, S.E.; Saphire-Bernstein, S.; Seeman, T.E. Are plasma oxytocin in women and plasma vasopressin in men biomarkers of distressed pair-bond relationships? Psychol. Sci. 2010, 21, 3–7, doi:10.1177/0956797609356507.
[30]  Ellis, L. Evidence of neuroandrogenic etiology of sex roles from a combined analysis of human, nonhuman primate and nonprimate mammalian studies. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 1986, 7, 519–552, doi:10.1016/0191-8869(86)90131-5.
[31]  Mazur, A. Biosociology of Dominance and Deference; Rowman & Littlefield Pub Incorporated: Oxford, UK, 2005.
[32]  Almeida, O.P.; Yeap, B.B.; Hankey, G.J.; Jamrozik, K.; Flicker, L. Low free testosterone concentration as a potentially treatable cause of depressive symptoms in older men. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2008, 65, 283, doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.33.
[33]  Carré, J.M.; Campbell, J.A.; Lozoya, E.; Goetz, S.M.; Welker, K.M. Changes in testosterone mediate the effect of winning on subsequent aggressive behaviour. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2013. in press.
[34]  Coste, S.C.; Murray, S.E.; Stenzel-Poore, M.P. Animal models of crh excess and crh receptor deficiency display altered adaptations to stress. Peptides 2001, 22, 733–741, doi:10.1016/S0196-9781(01)00386-2.
[35]  Selye, H. The Stress of Life; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1956; Volume 5.
[36]  Carter, S.C.; Porges, S.W. The neurobiology of social bonding and attachment. In The Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience; Decety, J., Cacioppo, J.T., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 151–163.
[37]  Kosfeld, M.; Heinrichs, M.; Zak, P.J.; Fischbacher, U.; Fehr, E. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 2005, 435, 673–676, doi:10.1038/nature03701.
[38]  Heinrichs, M.; von Dawans, B.; Domes, G. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2009, 30, 548, doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.005.
[39]  Thompson, R.; George, K.; Walton, J.; Orr, S.; Benson, J. Sex-specific influences of vasopressin on human social communication. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2006, 103, 7889–7894, doi:10.1073/pnas.0600406103.
[40]  Hokanson, J.E.; Willers, K.; Koropsak, E. The modification of autonomic responses during aggressive interchange1. J. Pers. 1968, 36, 386–404, doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1968.tb01481.x.
[41]  Taylor, S.E.; Klein, L.C.; Lewis, B.P.; Gruenewald, T.L.; Gurung, R.A.; Updegraff, J.A. Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychol. Rev. 2000, 107, 411–429, doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.411.
[42]  Ekman, P.; Friesen, W.V. Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1971, 17, 124–129, doi:10.1037/h0030377.
[43]  Ekman, P. All emotions are basic. In The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions; Ekman, P., Davidson, R.J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1994; pp. 15–19.
[44]  DeCatanzaro, D. Motivation and Emotion: Evolutionary, Physiological, Developmental, and Social Perspectives; Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 1999.
[45]  Weisfeld, G.E. Discrete emotions theory with specific reference to pride and shame. In Unitinig Psychology and Biology: Integrative Perspectives on Human Development; Segal, N.L., Weisfeld, G.E., Weisfeld, C.C., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1997; pp. 419–443.
[46]  James, W. II.—What is an emotion? Mind 1884, ox-IX, 188–205.
[47]  Lange, C.G. The mechanism of the emotions. In The Emotions; Dunlap, D., Ed.; Williams & Wilkins: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1885; pp. 33–92.
[48]  Rolls, E.T. The Brain and Emotion; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999.
[49]  Panksepp, J. The clearest physiological distintions between emotions will be found among the circuits of the brain. In The Nature of Emotion: Fundamenal Questions; Ekman, P., Davidson, R.J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1994; pp. 258–260.
[50]  Cannon, W.B. The James-Lange theory of emotions: A critical examination and an alternative theory. Am. J. Psychol. 1927, 39, 106–124, doi:10.2307/1415404.
[51]  Bard, P. A diencephalic mechanism for the expression of rage with special reference to the sympathetic nervous system. Am. J. Physiol. 1928, 84, 490–515.
[52]  Cacioppo, J.T.; Berntson, G.G.; Larsen, J.T.; Poehlmann, K.M.; Ito, T.A. The psychophysiology of emotion. In The Handbook of Emotion, 2nd ed.; Lewis, R., Haviland-Jones, J.M., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 173–191.
[53]  Cobos, P.; Sánchez, M.; Garc??a, C.; Nieves Vera, M.; Vila, J. Revisiting the james versus cannon debate on emotion: Startle and autonomic modulation in patients with spinal cord injuries. Biol. Psychol. 2002, 61, 251–269, doi:10.1016/S0301-0511(02)00061-3.
[54]  Hohmann, G.W. Some effects of spinal cord lesions on experienced emotional feelings. Psychophysiology 1966, 3, 143–156, doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1966.tb02690.x.
[55]  Laird, J.D. Self-attribution of emotion: The effects of expressive behavior on the quality of emotional experience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1974, 29, 475–486, doi:10.1037/h0036125.
[56]  Lanzetta, J.T.; Cartwright-Smith, J.; Kleck, R.E. Effects of nonverbal dissimulation on emotional experience and autonomic arousal. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1976, 33, 354–370, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.33.3.354.
[57]  Buck, R. Nonverbal behavior and the theory of emotion: The facial feedback hypothesis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1980, 38, 811–824, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.38.5.811.
[58]  Keillor, J.M.; Barrett, A.M.; Crucian, G.P.; Kortenkamp, S.; Heilman, K.M. Emotional experience and perception in the absence of facial feedback. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2002, 8, 130–135, doi:10.1017/S1355617702811134.
[59]  Calder, A.J.; Lawrence, A.D.; Keane, J.; Scott, S.K.; Owen, A.M.; Christoffels, I.; Young, A.W. Reading the mind from eye gaze. Neuropsychologia 2002, 40, 1129–1138, doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00008-8.
[60]  Smith, M.C.; Smith, M.K.; Ellgring, H. Spontaneous and posed facial expression in parkinson’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1996, 2, 383–391, doi:10.1017/S1355617700001454.
[61]  Kalat, J. North Carolina State University: Raleigh, NC, USA, 2013.
[62]  LeDoux, J.E. The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life; Simon and Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 1996.
[63]  MacLean, P.D. The Triune Brain in Evolution: Role in Paleocerebral Functions; Plenum Publishing Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1990.
[64]  Jarrell, T.W.; Gentile, C.G.; Romanski, L.M.; McCabe, P.M.; Schneiderman, N. Involvement of cortical and thalamic auditory regions in retention of differential bradycardiac conditioning to acoustic conditioned stimuli in rabbits. Brain Res. 1987, 412, 285–294, doi:10.1016/0006-8993(87)91135-8.
[65]  Damasio, A. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain; Penguin Books: New York, NY, USA, 2005.
[66]  Dunn, B.D.; Dalgleish, T.; Lawrence, A.D. The somatic marker hypothesis: A critical evaluation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2006, 30, 239–271, doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.07.001.
[67]  Maia, T.V.; McClelland, J.L. A reexamination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis: What participants really know in the iowa gambling task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2004, 101, 16075–16080, doi:10.1073/pnas.0406666101.
[68]  Barrash, J.; Tranel, D.; Anderson, S.W. Acquired personality disturbances associated with bilateral damage to the ventromedial prefrontal region. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2000, 18, 355–381, doi:10.1207/S1532694205Barrash.
[69]  Fellows, L.K.; Farah, M.J. Different underlying impairments in decision-making following ventromedial and dorsolateral frontal lobe damage in humans. Cereb. Cortex 2005, 15, 58–63, doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh108.
[70]  Heims, H.; Critchley, H.; Dolan, R.; Mathias, C.; Cipolotti, L. Social and motivational functioning is not critically dependent on feedback of autonomic responses: Neuropsychological evidence from patients with pure autonomic failure. Neuropsychologia 2004, 42, 1979–1988, doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.06.001.
[71]  Panksepp, J. Looking for spinoza: Joy, sorrow, and the feelign brain. Conscious. Emot. 2003, 4, 111–134, doi:10.1075/ce.4.1.10pan.
[72]  Liotti, M.; Tucker, D.M. Emotion in asymmetric corticolimbic networks. In Brain Asymmetry; Davidson, R.J., Hugdahl, K., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995; pp. 389–423.
[73]  Schachter, S.; Singer, J.E. Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychol. Rev. 1962, 69, 379–399, doi:10.1037/h0046234.
[74]  Lindquist, K.A.; Wager, T.D.; Kober, H.; Bliss-Moreau, E.; Barrett, L.F. The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Behav. Brain Sci. 2012, 35, 121–143, doi:10.1017/S0140525X11000446.
[75]  Dethier, V.G.; Clark, B. To Know a Fly; Holden-Day: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1962.
[76]  Carroll, S.B. Endless Forms Most Beautiful: The New Science of Evo Devo and the Making of the Animal Kingdom; WW Norton & Company: New York, NY, USA, 2005.
[77]  Vytal, K.; Hamann, S. Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A voxel-based meta-analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010, 22, 2864–2885, doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21366.
[78]  Phan, K.L.; Wager, T.; Taylor, S.F.; Liberzon, I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in pet and fmri. Neuroimage 2002, 16, 331–348, doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1087.
[79]  Murphy, F.C.; Nimmo-Smith, I.; Lawrence, A.D. Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: A meta-analysis. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2003, 3, 207–233, doi:10.3758/CABN.3.3.207.
[80]  Hamann, S. Mapping discrete and dimensional emotions onto the brain: Controversies and consensus. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 458–466, doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.07.006.
[81]  Bartels, A.; Zeki, S. The neural basis of romantic love. Neuroreport 2000, 11, 3829–3834, doi:10.1097/00001756-200011270-00046.
[82]  Coons, E.E.; Levak, M.; Miller, N.E. Lateral hypothalamus: Learning of food-seeking response motivated by electrical stimulation. Science 1965, 150, 1320–1321.
[83]  Weaver, D.R.; Rivkees, S.A.; Reppert, S.M. Localization and characterization of melatonin receptors in rodent brain by in vitro autoradiography. J. Neurosci. 1989, 9, 2581–2590.
[84]  Zink, C.F.; Barter, J.W. Neural representation of social hierarchy. In The Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience; Decety, J., Cacioppo, J.T., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 702–715.
[85]  Olds, J.; Milner, P. Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1954, 47, 419–427, doi:10.1037/h0058775.
[86]  Winkelman, P.; Berridge, K.; Sher, S. Emotion, consciousness, and social behavior. In The Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience; Decety, J., Cacioppo, J.T., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 195–211.
[87]  King, H.E. Psychological effects of excitation in the limbic system. In Electrical Stimulation of the Brain; Sheer, D.E., Ed.; University of Texas Press: Austin, TX, USA, 1961; pp. 477–486.
[88]  Gloor, P. The Temporal Lobe and Limbic System; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997.
[89]  Black, D.W. Pathological laughter: A review of the literature. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1982, 170, 67–71, doi:10.1097/00005053-198202000-00001.
[90]  Martin, J.P. Fits of laughter (sham mirth) in organic cerebral disease. Brain 1950, 73, 453–464, doi:10.1093/brain/73.4.453.
[91]  Feinstein, J.S.; Adolphs, R.; Damasio, A.; Tranel, D. The human amygdala and the induction and experience of fear. Curr. Biol. 2011, 21, 34–38, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.042.
[92]  MacLean, P.D. Cerebral evolution of emotion. In Handbook of Emotion; Lewis, M., Haviland, J.M., Eds.; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 1993; pp. 67–83.
[93]  LaFrenière, P. Adaptive Origins: Evolution and Human Development; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010.
[94]  Sroufe, L.A. Emotional Development: The Organization of Emotional Life in the Early Years; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1997.
[95]  LaFreniere, P.J. Emotional Development: A Biosocial Perspective; Wadsworth/Thomson Learning: Belmont, CA, USA, 2000.
[96]  Buss, K.A.; Goldsmith, H.H. Fear and anger regulation in infancy: Effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expression. Child Dev. 1998, 69, 359–374.
[97]  Egas, M.; Riedl, A. The economics of altruistic punishment and the maintenance of cooperation. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2008, 275, 871–878, doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1558.
[98]  Trivers, R.L. The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q. Rev. Biol. 1971, 46, 35–57.
[99]  Weisfeld, G.E. Social dominance and human motivation. In Dominance Relations: An Etholgoical View of Human Conflict and Social Interaction; Omark, D.R., Strayer, F.F., Freedman, D.G., Eds.; Garland: New York, NY, USA, 1980; pp. 273–286.
[100]  Harbaugh, W.T.; Mayr, U.; Burghart, D.R. Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science 2007, 316, 1622–1625, doi:10.1126/science.1140738.
[101]  Rilling, J.K.; Gutman, D.A.; Zeh, T.R.; Pagnoni, G.; Berns, G.S.; Kilts, C.D. A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron 2002, 35, 395–405, doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00755-9.
[102]  De Quervain, D.J.-F.; Fischbacher, U.; Treyer, V.; Schellhammer, M.; Schnyder, U.; Buck, A.; Fehr, E. The neural basis of altruistic punishment. Science 2004, 305, 1254–1258.
[103]  Averill, J.R. Studies on anger and aggression. Am. Psychol. 1983, 38, 1145–1160, doi:10.1037/0003-066X.38.11.1145.
[104]  Fehr, E.; G?chter, S. Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 2002, 415, 137–140, doi:10.1038/415137a.
[105]  Pastore, N. The role of arbitrariness in the frustration-aggression hypothesis. J. Abnor. Soc. Psych. 1952, 47, 728–731, doi:10.1037/h0060884.
[106]  Seip, E.C.; van Dijk, W.W.; Rotteveel, M. On hotheads and dirty harries. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2009, 1167, 190–196, doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04503.x.
[107]  Singer, T.; Seymour, B.; O’Doherty, J.P.; Stephan, K.E.; Dolan, R.J.; Frith, C.D. Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 2006, 439, 466–469, doi:10.1038/nature04271.
[108]  Ohbuchi, K.-I.; Kameda, M.; Agarie, N. Apology as aggression control: Its role in mediating appraisal of and response to harm. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 56, 219, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.219.
[109]  De Oliveira-Souza, R.; Hare, R.D.; Bramati, I.E.; Garrido, G.J.; Azevedo Ignácio, F.; Tovar-Moll, F.; Moll, J. Psychopathy as a disorder of the moral brain: Fronto-temporo-limbic grey matter reductions demonstrated by voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage 2008, 40, 1202–1213, doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.054.
[110]  Beer, J.S. Neural systems of intrapersonal and interpersonal self-esteem maintenance. In The Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience; Decety, J., Cacioppo, J.T., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 599–613.
[111]  Krajbich, I.; Adolphs, R.; Tranel, D.; Denburg, N.L.; Camerer, C.F. Economic games quantify diminished sense of guilt in patients with damage to the prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2009, 29, 2188–2192, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5086-08.2009.
[112]  Barbey, A.K.; Grafman, J. The prefrontal cortex and goal-directed social behavior. In The Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience; Decety, J., Cacioppo, J.T., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 348–359.
[113]  Weisfeld, G.E. Humor appreciation as an adaptive esthetic emotion. Humor 2006, 19, 1–26, doi:10.1515/HUMOR.2006.001.
[114]  Weisfeld, G.E. The adaptive value of humor and laughter. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1993, 14, 141–169, doi:10.1016/0162-3095(93)90012-7.
[115]  Tinbergen, N. The Study of Instinct; Clarendon Press: Oxford, UK, 1951; p. 228.
[116]  Rausch, S.L.; Shin, L.M.; Wright, C.I. Neuroimaging studies of amygdala functino in anxiety disorders. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2003, 983, 389–410.

Full-Text

  
  
  

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133